An introduction to developmental problems in puppy hood

01 July 2013
19 mins read
Volume 4 · Issue 6

Abstract

Many veterinary staff wondered why the Companion Animal Welfare Council, that conducted independent studies into the welfare, care and treatment of companion animals, concluded that the minimum standard of accredited qualification for professionals instructing a puppy class should be at level 4 to 5, i.e. equivalent to a foundation degree or early years of undergraduate level study. It is intended that this article will go some way towards explaining the complexity and variety of problems that can be faced by the young puppy and why it is essential for veterinary staff in charge of this part of a dog's emotional and behavioural development to be able to recognise problems and give appropriate advice to owners. To fail to intervene appropriately in this essential aspect of preventative behavioural medicine can lead to permanent and irreversible behavioural problems. In turn such problems may lead to abuse, relinquishment or euthanasia of the dog and to potential injury to owners. Put simply, there is more to preparing puppies for life than opportunities to play together and it is particularly inappropriate to encourage a party atmosphere of excessive emotional arousal.

It must be at least 20 years since enlightened veterinary practices began to use Scott and Fuller's (1965) research to inform their work in introducing puppies to the veterinary environment and the world at large. These puppy classes became increasingly popular, becoming known as ‘puppy parties’ due to the opportunities for puppies to play together, and the upbeat title seemed to encourage owners to engage with the concept of attending the gatherings. Despite the gradual spread of practice puppy classes, behaviour counsellors continue to encounter clients with dogs for whom a lack of appropriate early exposure to the world forms a predisposing factor in the development of their presenting behaviour problem. More worrying is the increasing number of dogs presenting with such a predisposing factor, who have attended puppy classes (Seksel, 2012a) and despite the current popularity of veterinary run puppy classes, behaviour problems remain the most common reason for canine euthanasia reducing the average age of dogs to 3.5 years (Seksel, 2012b). If puppy classes are being run with the specific aim of improving canine welfare, something must be going wrong! This article examines some of the influences that create behavioural and emotional problems for the young dog and their families, yet may be accidentally overlooked in veterinary led puppy classes. It is not intended as a guide to what should be done within puppy classes, but it is hoped that it will encourage veterinary staff to think more laterally when encountering puppies who seem to fail to fit into the concept of what is normal behavioural development.

Nature or nurture?

The 19th and 20th century psychologists argued long and hard over the relative influences of nature and nurture on an individual's behaviour. The current consensus is that both the genetic background (including evolutionary, innate influences) and the environment (both previous experiences and the current situation that the animal finds itself in) influence an animal's behavioural response. It is also acknowledged that every individual is different, as even genetically identical twins will have had slightly different experiences that will have informed their individual behavioural choices (Jensen, 2010). As a consequence of this individuality, it is difficult to imagine how professionals running puppy classes can assume that every puppy will be the same, and that the same advice and techniques will meet the individual needs of each member of the puppy group within a class. Anyone running a puppy class needs to have a basic understanding of the many influences that may alter a puppy's capacity to deal with the world around it. The following are an introduction to the variety of in-fluences that may need to be considered.

Genetic effects

It is often difficult to discuss the direct genetic influences that a puppy may be subject to — although owners may have been lucky enough to have seen their puppy's pedigree and met the parents of their puppy, it can be unwise to make assumptions about direct inheritance of characteristics from parents without direct proof. However, due to many generations of evidence it is far more reliable to make assumptions associated with evolutionary effects and innate responses such as those mentioned below. It should be noted that, as young puppies that have had limited exposure to the world, their behavioural responses will rely largely on those behaviours that are innate to their species. Opportunities to modulate these innate responses can only come through the puppy experiencing learning that enables them to understand that behavioural alternatives to innate responses may bring about more successful outcomes regarding their capacity to cope in their environment (Overall, 2009) (Figure 1).

Figure 1. By ensuring that puppies play in pairs rather than larger groups inappropriate activities that could lead to detrimental learning could be interrupted.

Breed effects

Selective breeding has resulted in genotypic differences and phenotypic differences that have produced an array of breed differences within the single species Canis familiaris.

Although originally intended to enable the selection of dogs with specific abilities such as hunting and herding, most differences also show themselves in anatomical traits affecting ears, tails and noses. These anatomical changes have not altered behaviours associated with self preservation or sexual behaviour, which remain common to all breeds (Lindsay, 2000), but some traits have resulted in anatomical changes that make canine communication and signalling difficult for both canines and humans to read. The positions of ears and the facial expression of dogs are particularly important in signalling the progression of emotional changes. The position of the tail is also indicative of emotional state. The effectiveness of such communication tools can be compromised by breed characteristics such as ears that are incapable of standing erect, brachycephalic conformations that reveal teeth, hair that covers eyes, tails that are constantly held erect, piloerection along the backbone.

Brain receptor sensitivity

Both genetics and inheritance affect brain receptor sensitivity, and traits such as fearfulness and anxiety may be partially inherited (Mills, 2005). Differences in emotional thresholds can be affected by a limbic/autonomic inheritance present at birth (Lindsay, 2000), and the dog's general emotional reactivity or threshold to stimulation will be a pre-disposing factor towards the development of behaviour problems. Many studies have shown emotional extremes involving anxiety and fear are highly hereditable (Thorne, 1944), and such dogs are much more sensitive to environmental stimulation and this sensitivity can be difficult to alter despite the influence of learning in later life.

Breed and behaviour

Breed standards have developed that not only specify the type of work that the dog should be able to perform, but also desired behavioural traits such as ‘wary’, ‘cautious’ or ‘reserved’. Strong genetic links to emotionality leading to behaviour problems have been demonstrated in specific lines of certain breeds of dogs (Podeberscek and Serpell, 1996 and 1997) and to the symptoms of anxiety and fear (Scott and Fuller, 1960; Scherrer et al, 2000).

Inherited temperament traits influence the way dogs learn and how they are most effectively trained, and various behavioural profiles have been prepared to assist potential dog owners in selecting a breed that is compatible with their lifestyle (e.g. Scott and Fuller, 1965 and Hart and Hart, 1985); the Waltham Centre for Pet Nutrition has a counselling service assisting potential owners in finding the ideal companion. All of these scientific and applied activities are based on the assumption that a considerable portion of a dog's behaviour is affected by inherited tendencies and traits.

Breed effects relating to species-specific responses

Although not completely reliable due to the potential for individual variation in the species-specific response following from opportunities to learn about the result of responses, breeding is likely to dictate the initial expression of a dog's species-specific response. In the presence of a fear-inducing stimulus and with sufficient behavioural freedom to do so, dogs of most breeds would respond by fleeing from the stimulus. However this is not the case for all breeds. The development of specific breeds for specific purposes may have interfered with the species-specific response, as is the case with terrier breeds that have been specifically bred to initiate aggression on threat, and with the beagle that is unusual in its tendency to freeze when threatened (Scott and Fuller, 1965). This genetic pre-disposition to express a specific response when faced with challenging situations can make it extremely difficult for a dog (e.g. terrier) to respond in a manner that the general public may consider socially appropriate.

Stimuli that may induce emotions such as fear or anxiety in the dog

As already alluded to, anxious or fearful responses are not always learned or acquired through experience. Fear is often evoked by unconditioned aversive stimuli that have evolutionary significance, known as innate fear stimuli. Different species show biological preparedness to different sets of such stimuli. For dogs, innate fear stimuli include pain, rapid stimulus change, loud noises and other intense sensory stimulation, sudden movements, heights, isolation, fire, water, and unfamiliar environmental and social situations (i.e. novel environmental or social stimuli such as unknown people). This latter trigger may appear incongruous for a social species but there is a tendency to forget that within a ‘natural’ dog group the social composition would remain fairly constant throughout an individual dog's life.

Natural triggers of fear are associated with imminent threat and evoke preparatory physiological arousal mediating species-specific escape responses.

Inter-uterine influences on puppy behaviour

Maternal stress

Studies of rodents and other altricial species (species in which the young are born in an imature state, e.g. kitten, puppy or rat) indicate that transplacental maternal influences may affect the subsequent behaviour of the offspring (Thompson, 1957) with females that were subjected to stressful experiences during pregnancy producing offspring that were emotional or reactive in test situations, independent of genetic influences (Takahashi and Kalim, 1991). In an altricial species the immature state of the foetal nervous system makes it unlikely that prenatal effects on behaviour can result from learning, and such changes in emotionality are probably due to the direct effect of maternal corticosteroid hormones on the development of the foetus' physiological response to stress. Stressful experiences during pregnancy may render offspring more reactive to stress (Serpell and Jagoe, 1995). It has been reported that relaxed bitches that receive affectionate contact from humans during pregnancy produce litters that are much more tolerant of touching and patting compared with bitches that were given no petting (Dehasse, 1994). However, research indicates that prenatal stress produces an increased behavioral responsiveness to stress that is evident in early life and continues into adulthood (Takahashi et al, 1992,Weinstock, 2001 and 2008). Studies also show that prenatal stress can pre-dispose animals to depressive psychological conditions in adulthood (Weinstock, 2005).

Sex hormones

At the stage of foetal development when the central nervous system is most sensitive to androgens, testo-sterone is only produced by male foetuses and causes masculinisation of their brains. It has been found that in litter-bearing species, such as the dog, some female foetuses may become exposed to testosterone, particularly if the foetus lies between two males in the uterine horn. Testosterone may diffuse through the amniotic membranes or enter the foetal blood circulation (vom Saal, 1989) causing similar developmental changes in the female brain as those occurring in the surrounding males. These masculinised or androgenised females may show more male-like behaviour than non-masculinised females, including an increased tendency to exhibit aggression (Beach, 1974; Brain and Haug, 1992; Overall, 1995), also leg-lifting to urinate. Foetal androgenisation may increase the incidence in the development of resource-based aggression in bitches after spaying (Voith and Borchelt, 1982; Warnes, 2005) once the ameliorating effects of oestrogen and progesterone are reduced. There may be a gradation of masculinisation depending on the level of exposure to testosterone (Overall, 1995)

It has also been suggested that male puppies, positioned in utero between other male puppies, may be exposed to increased levels of testosterone that enhance the development of primary sexually dimorphic behaviours such as rough play and increased activity. Punishment of such seemingly excessive behavioural displays may lead to owners inadvertently initiating a motivation for defensive behaviour in the young dog, leading to unintended learned associations.

Postnatal changes

The dam and nest

There are some anecdotal reports that the mother may play an important role in the regulation of aggressive behaviour through the exercise of early discipline (Lindsay, 2000). It is also widely accepted that modelling and observational learning occurs so that the mother's emotional tone and reactivity may encourage similar reactions in puppies. The mother's negative reactions to social contacts or events may be the result of heredity, her own personal history, or a combination of both and impressionable puppies are at considerable risk of internalising her attitude during the socialisation period as well as before birth (Thompson, 1957). It is a common belief (Lindsay, 2000) that puppies reflect more of the mother's emotionality than the father's and prenatal and postnatal influences probably exert a more significant influence than sex-linkage in these apparent differences.

The influence of the dam's emotionality, both during and post pregnancy, on the developing emotional baseline of the puppies enhances the necessity for breeders (both professional and hobbyists) to ensure that potential mothers are capable of coping with potential emotionally arousing circumstances and that their environment is managed to reduce potential stressors. The stability of the social environment in which pregnant females live is critical for their offsprings' social behaviour later in life (Kaiser and Sacher, 2005).

The nest environment is in itself important to the emotional development of the puppy. Environments that are lacking in resources such as toys can have a two-fold negative effect on the puppies' developing behavioural repertoire. Barren nest sites enhance the salience of the mother as she is the only stimulus, social or otherwise, within the environment (Figure 2). This can lead to puppies finding it extremely difficult to cope with the environmental and social changes that occur as they enter their new home, enhancing the likelihood of distress, associated learning and separation problems. Barren early environments also pre-dispose puppies to competitive and resource holding behaviours, as if insufficient in number, those distracting items that are placed in the nest area become a focus for competitive behaviours rather than enhancing appropriate social behaviour including shared social play.

Figure 2. Puppies should be maintained with mum. Encouraging the puppies to spend time in an enriched and complex environment reduces the salience of their mother and enhances their capacity to cope with joining a new home.

Distress and impulsivity

Some external influences may have long-term effects on learning, emotionality and general adaptability despite the neonatal state of the puppy (between birth and 12 days). Early neonatal handling for as little as 3 minutes per day and exposure to mild environmental stressors such as slight changes in ambient temperature and gentle movement may have positive effects on a puppy's later emotional reactivity and mature learning and problem solving abilities (Morton, 1968). Animals left undisturbed during neonatal development have been found to be consistently more emotionally reactive as adults (Danenberg, 1964).

Perinatal brain plasticity increases the vulnerability to early adverse experiences, thus leading to abnormal development and behaviour (Anand and Scalzo, 2000). These changes may promote anxiety, altered pain sensitivity, stress disorders, hyperactivity/attention deficit disorder and are likely to lead to impaired social skills. Studies such as those undertaken in altricial species by de Kloet et al (2005) suggest that adverse conditions during early life are a risk factor for stress-related diseases (Figure 3) such as depression and post-traumatic stress disorder, and that adverse early experiences trigger immediate changes in the stress system that may permanently alter the brain and behaviour.

Figure 3. Emotional distress during the early months of development can lead to permanent and irreversible behavioural problems..

Socialisation

The development of a dog's social behaviour unfolds according to a genetically programmed timetable (Scott and Fuller, 1965) and this developmental process has a permanent influence on the behavioural adjustment of the dog (Lindsay, 2000). During the brief period between 3 to between 12 and 16 weeks (depending on breed) the average puppy will learn more than during the remaining course of its life, forming a lasting emotional and cognitive scheme of the social and physical environment. These early experiences produce the range of how and what the dog is prepared to experience and learn in the future.

Much of what is learned during this early period is lasting, providing the foundation for adult behaviour patterns and problems (Fox, 1969) including aversions, social affinities, patterns of active and passive agonistic behaviour, reactions to separation and other emotionally provocative situations, approach-avoidance patterns, patterns of exploratory behaviour and functional fear and avoidance responses — all of which have direct relevance to the development of the dog's behavioural repertoire, including the use of aggression.

A common source of anxiety, fearfulness and aggression is a lack of adequate socialisation and social referencing to the complexities of the social mix and diversity of stimuli in the environment in which the adult dog will have to live (McCune et al, 1995). Domesticity demands that the dog must display considerable social flexibility and the opportunity to learn about this generally occurs at 7 weeks plus as the puppy enters its new home. However, observation of livestock guarding dogs and their ability to form strong inter-specific relationships with species on which a less socialised dog would predate (Coppinger, 2002) and research on sensitive periods in behavioural development (Pluijmakers et al, 2003) would suggest that the learning that occurs between 3-6 weeks is essential if the dog is to become socially competent with a species other than its own. It may also be best to initiate environmental referencing during this period when puppies are more outgoing and less fearful than older puppies.

Social and environmental referencing

Between the ages of 3 and 16 weeks, access to an environment that is rich and diverse is essential for future emotional stability (Thompson and Heron, 1954). Although it is hoped that the breeding environment provides puppies with opportunities for exploratory behaviour they must also be exposed to the environment within which they will spend most of their life; this includes exposure to traffic and the sound of domestic appliances and routines.

Careful environmental exposure carried out systematically through gradual and incremental increases in intensity and duration, allows a puppy to habituate to potentially fear-eliciting stimuli. Classical conditioning, using an appetitive unconditioned stimulus associated with neutral stimuli such as being handled or placed on tables, can further enhance the animal's resistance to developing fear responses later in life.

Dishabituation

Social and environmental referencing occurs through a process of learning known as habituation. During habituation an animal learns to accept environmental and social stimuli that are found within the environment and the process will occur naturally on gentle exposure to stimuli that do not become associated with emotional arousal. As the puppy is more likely to be in a para-sympathetic state prior to 6 to 8 weeks of age, this process occurs most easily at this time.

However despite exposure to a wide range of social and environmental stimuli associated with the domestic environment, puppies that fail to continue experiencing regular exposure to such stimuli may become dishabituated and respond on subsequent exposure as though habituation had never occurred. The process of dishabituation is further enhanced by concurrent emotions of anxiety or fear. Consequently puppies that experience periods of environmental or social isolation on rehoming in combination with stress, e.g. if they are unable to continue exposure to the environment due to illness or kennelling, are particularly susceptible to dishabituation (Sherman and Mills, 2008).

Maturation and hormonal effects

Increased competitiveness and aggression is often associated with the hormonal changes occurring at puberty and these may lower the threshold for frustration and consequently for aggression, particularly for sex-related behaviour patterns such as aggression towards other dogs. It is interesting to note that as oestrus is approached, circulating testosterone in female dogs reaches plasma levels comparable to those in male dogs (Olson et al, 1984). Despite their recognised role in regulating the expression of intraspecific aggression, there is as yet no reliable research to implicate the direct role of sexual hormones in aggression towards humans or other species, with the possible exception of testosterone which has been frequently quoted as an aggression facilitating hormone (Lindsay, 2001). Although the general ineffectiveness of castration of male dogs in controlling aggressive behaviour may put this opinion in question. Behaviours associated with maintaining access to and control over resources may also increase as the young dog approaches sexual maturity (Overall, 1995, 1997, and 2003a and b).

Learning

Controlled studies (Scott and Fuller, 1965) show that puppies between the ages of 3 to 16 weeks are able learners and at no other time in the dog's life will it be more receptive to training, functioning at a nearly adult level in terms of learning ability. But although the puppy's ability to concentrate on tasks continues to increase, by 16 weeks the ease with which they learn begins to decline. This is probably due to previous learning beginning to interfere with new learning. This further emphasises the importance of the nature and quality of the puppy's early experience of its social and physical environment if aggressive behaviour is to be avoided.

Survival depends on an animal learning from past experiences, adjusting its behaviour appropriately to current circumstances, and forming reliable predictions about similar situations in the future. Consequently, learning has an important role in the development of anxieties and fears. There is a temptation to consider behaviour as either innate or learned; however, learning cooperates with innate mechanisms to ensure that organisms are well adapted to the environment in which they must survive.

Classical conditioning and chaining

Anxiety and fear are learned through classical conditioning. Learning that is classically conditioned involves an involuntary or reflex response and it does not rely on the presence of external reward. An example within the veterinary practice would be where a dog has learned to become fearful (unconditioned response) when given pain through being injected (unconditional stimulus). The sound of a syringe or needle packet being opened may become the conditioned stimulus (CS), leading to signs of fear before the injection is given. As learning is intended to maintain the safety of the individual, the animal will quickly chain events backwards from the original CS to other preceding, predictive stimuli to which the animal may also show the conditioned response of fear. In this case the chain could begin with a drawer opening, but on the next occasion when being placed on the examination table, then the dog may show signs of fear towards a person in a white coat in the examination room. This may then generalise to anyone in a white coat and even to anyone wearing white.

If the original stimuli are sufficiently salient or if the animal is already in a state of emotional arousal or anxiety before the presentation of the stimuli, classical associations may be learned in a single exposure (Lindsay, 2000).

Training

Most of what a dog does can be interpreted in terms of innate behavioural predispositions manifested under the influence of learning (Lindsay, 2000). As long as the dog exhibits sufficient variability and flexibility in its innately prepared and instinctive behaviour, that behaviour can be modified through training.

The success of training is likely to be dependent on the dog's previous learning opportunities, the motivation to perform the new skills in preference to previous responses (which may be coping strategies that the dog considers to be necessary within the circumstances), the method of training used and the context in which the trained behaviour is requested. Training may not automatically control a dog's behaviour in a specific context until the dog has had opportunities to become competent in performing the behaviour, to discriminate the cue from background stimuli, by generalising the behaviour to a variety of contexts, including within that specific environment.

Frustration

Frustration, or failure to gain access to the expected reward, is a common factor in mammalian emotions and frequently results in distress.

Many behaviourists believe that hand-reared puppies are particularly prone to this problem due to a failure to go through a normal weaning process that involves the puppies learning that the immediate satiation of a need for resources is not guaranteed. However, puppies do not have to be hand reared to miss this stage in their social learning — puppies that go straight from feeding on their mother to semi-solid food (possibly supplemented with occasional opportunities for suckling) effectively fail to experience periods of having to ‘do without’.

Some puppies enter their new home without experiencing a previous need to engage enhanced emotional activity in response to a delayed response to their perception of their needs (frustration). Initially many owners will pander to the puppy's every need, but they eventually fail to be present at the initiation of ‘need’, inadvertently initiating incidents of frustration.

At such times, frustration bursts of puppy activity aimed at gaining access to a resource that is perceived to be required to satiate the puppy's ‘need’ will be initiated. If owners comply with the puppy's requirements the behaviours associated with the frustration burst will be reinforced for future use. If owners fail to comply, the associated distress my result in an escalation of behaviour and possibly aggression. As with all aggressive incidents in puppies, the manner in which the owner handles such outbursts may have a permanent effect on pet-owner relationships and any attempt by owners to use aversive control techniques will merely enhance the puppy's perception of threat and the need for defensive behaviour (Arhanta et al, 2010).

Fear, anxiety and the environment

Both fear (a stress initiating negative emotional response to a specific stimulus) and anxiety (a stress initiating emotional response associated with an anticipatory state) produce a need within the individual to create an area of safety around it (Appleby, 1997). A fearful or anxious dog will signal that it is uncomfortable with the proximity of a stimulus by initially barking and if this is not successful by growling and eventually biting. A failure to reduce the degree of stimulation will escalate the signalling and possibly teach the dog that the initial stages of this sequence are pointless as only snapping or biting actually achieves the dog's intended result of removing the problem stimulus (Askew, 1996).

The interaction between emotional responses such as fear and anxiety and the resulting behaviour is influenced by:

  • Learning and the impact of emotional changes on the process of learning
  • The normal species-specific differences in behavioural responses to fear and anxiety
  • The environmental stimuli to which each species may be predisposed to respond.
  • For each individual the breed, sex, age, experience during sensitive periods for learning, previous learning experiences, physical and social contexts in which the behaviour occurs, opportunities for generalisation and reinforcement and the consequence of the behaviour for the individual will all play a role in the development of fear or anxiety.

    Anxiety, fear and separation problems

    Fone and Porkess (2008) suggested that exposing mammals to early-life adverse events, including maternal separation or social isolation, profoundly affects brain development and adult behaviour. In addition, anxiety and fear may enhance the salience of the presence of an attachment figure, as puppies find that ‘coping’ is easier at such times. As a con-sequence, adverse events not only enhance fear and anxiety in an increasing array of situations, but they also predispose the young dog to distress and to separation-related problems (Appleby and Pluijmakers, 2004).

    Distress and impulsivity

    Impulsivity is a common factor in puppy behaviour — the inability to understand the likely outcome of behaviours — a particular problem when puppies have not yet learned the capacity for self control. However, not all puppies learn to control their excessive out-bursts of behaviour and impulsivity in adult dogs may result from brain changes that occur early in life as a result of early stress.

    Human psychiatric studies show that early abusive experiences can create brain changes resulting in the impulsivity associated with adult psychiatric conditions. These patients cannot be cured, but they can be helped to manage these behaviours — to identify triggers and to avoid them. However, it is impossible to create such learning in a non-human animal. You cannot teach a dog to stop being impulsive; it is the responsibility of the owner to recognise trigger factors and environments and to manage them appropriately so that the dog does not experience the need to engage in impulsivity.

    Setting up a clinic

    It was not intended that this article should provide a basis on which puppy classes should be run. Many veterinary practices have been providing puppy education in an innovative fashion that encourages owners (even those with considerable previous experience) to engage with the wealth of new ideas that are continually evolving. For those wishing to investigate this topic further there is some excellent materials being produced by the university of Lincoln's Behaviour Department that can assist veterinary nurses in designing a comprehensive package of support for puppies and their families. The recently initiated animal behaviour and training council which evolved from the Companion Animal Welfare Council (CAWC) process gives details of the academic levels to which those involved in the preparation of puppies for domestic life should be competent. The following website provides information on the minimum academic level essential when providing a full service to support the emotional development and behaviour of the young dog — www.abtcouncil.org.uk. Any courses taken should be appropriately accredited to the suggested academic level.

    Conclusion

    Responsibility for the behavioural welfare of canine patients starts with the dog's first contact with the practice. From that point, the advice given to owners needs to be accurate and specific to the individual patient. Every member of the veterinary practice's staff should be able to identify a dog that is failing to cope in the veterinary environment and refer owners to a staff member who can assist with advice. However, due to individual differences in the patients' preparation for domestic life and subsequent learning, only appropriately qualified members of staff should issue preventative and rehabilitation advice to clients.

    Puppy classes are not a part of the practice's service that can be entrusted to enthusiastic but inexpert staff — the recognition and immediate resolution of puppy problems is a specialised job that should only be undertaken by those appropriately trained to do so. A puppy class instructor needs to be able to identify a puppy or owner that is failing to cope and give immediate and specific preventative or remedial advice. The instructor should be sufficiently confident to recognise when an owner's needs exceeds the instructor's level of competence and they should be ready to refer the puppy on to a veterinary behaviourist or certified clinical animal behaviourist. When practices fail to recognise and take the lead in the specialist aspect of puppy behavioural welfare, they inadvertently ensure that some puppies develop behaviours that inevitably lead to life-threatening crisis — possibly irreversible — for both dog and family.

    Key points

  • Many puppy classes concentrate on ‘fun’ to the detriment of puppies learning to control their emotions.
  • The early months of the puppy's life takes it through the periods of infant, juvenile and young adult developmental phases.
  • Dogs have not evolved with the innate skills to relax in the complex human environment and consequently frequently fail to cope.
  • Species-specific and breed effects often impede the young dog's capacity to cope in a human environment.
  • Physical or emotional distress experienced during puppy hood can have permanent affects on emotional and behavioural developments that later training will not alter.
  • Introducing puppies to the veterinary environment through calm, well-structured classes gives veterinary staff better opportunities to identify and assist puppies that are failing to cope.